Menu

Waiting Rarely Works: Late Bloomers Usually Just Wilt

A look at three pivotal longitudinal studies that clearly show: Late bloomers are rare; skill deficits are almost always what prevent children from blooming as readers.

For thirty years, up until about a decade ago, the idea of "late bloomers" was widely believed among researchers and educators alike. "Late bloomer" was the endearing term for a child who was slower than his peers in learning to read. The idea, so well captured in the term, was that these children would bloom in their reading—they would just do it a bit later than their peers. This common view, known among researchers as the "developmental lag" theory, was the reasonable basis for teachers' patience with students who didn't catch on to reading quickly—and it justified the common practice of delaying the diagnosis of reading problems until they were quite severe (Lyon et al., 2001).

But more recently, long after many teachers ended their formal education training, researchers have been able to put the developmental lag theory to rest. It has been replaced by an alternate theory of early reading weakness that defines the problem as a skill deficit. The main difference between the two theories is that the developmental lag theory posited that difficulties in learning to read would fade as the brain matured—early, urgent intervention was not necessary. In contrast, the skill deficit theory claimed that waiting wouldn't work; children wouldn't pick up these skills unless they were taught directly and intensively. In fact, waiting would be harmful, as it condemned children to falling further behind.

Three longitudinal studies (Juel, 1988; Francis et al., 1996; Shaywitz et al., 1999) have put the weight of research squarely behind the skill deficit theory and against the developmental lag theory. Each study tracked the reading development of children beginning in first grade.

In the simplest terms, these studies ask: Do struggling readers catch up? The data from the studies are clear: Late bloomers are rare; skill deficits are almost always what prevent children from blooming as readers. This research may be counter-intuitive to elementary teachers who have seen late-bloomers in their own classes or heard about them from colleagues. But statistically speaking, such students are rare. (Actually, as we'll see, there is nearly a 90 percent chance that a poor reader in first grade will remain a poor reader.)

The first study (Juel 1988) tracked 54 children at a school in Austin, Texas, from the beginning of first grade through the end of fourth grade using a variety of standardized tests of phonemic awareness, decoding, word recognition, listening comprehension, and reading comprehension. To see if those who are behind in learning to read do or do not catch up, Juel split the students into two groups based on their scores at the end of first grade on the ITBS Reading Comprehension subtest. Those who scored in the bottom quartile (based on national norms) were labeled "poor readers." Those in the top three quartiles were labeled "average or good readers."

Over the next three years, the poor readers, on average, never caught up to the average and good readers on any measure of reading ability. Consider, for example, the two groups' grade-level equivalents on the ITBS Reading Comprehension subtest at the end of first grade and at the end of fourth grade. The poor readers' mean score increased from K6 (a mid-kindergarten level) to 3.5 (a mid-third grade level). But the average and good readers' mean score increased from a 2.4 to a 5.9.

Of course, group averages don't reveal individual results. Were there some late bloomers hidden behind these means? Not many. On the ITBS Reading Comprehension subtest, students who score in the bottom quartile at the end of first grade are, in terms of grade-level equivalents, at least six months behind. So Juel examined the individual results at the end of fourth grade to see how many students were still at least six months behind. Of the 24 students who were poor readers in first grade, 21 of them were still at least six months behind in reading. Similarly, of the 30 students who were average or good readers at the end of first grade, only four had fallen six or more months behind. Juel summarized her findings as follows:

The probability that a child would remain a poor reader at the end of fourth grade, if the child was a poor reader at the end of first grade, was .88; the probability that a child would become a poor reader in fourth grade if he or she had at least average reading skills in first grade was .12. The probability that a child would remain an average reader in fourth grade if the child had average reading ability in first grade was .87; the probability that a child would become an average reader in the fourth grade if he or she was a poor reader in first grade was only .13. The evidence in this sample of children indicates that the poor first-grade reader almost invariably remains a poor reader by the end of fourth grade. (Juel, 1988)

Furthermore, Juel found that the poor readers lacked a critical skill: phonemic awareness. The poor readers entered first grade with little phonemic awareness and they did not approach the ceiling on the phonemic awareness test until the end of third grade. In contrast, average and good readers approached the ceiling on that test two years earlier, at the end of first grade. She concluded that it was trouble with decoding, rooted in poor phonemic awareness, that appeared to keep the poor readers from improving. With this finding, Juel did much to boost the case of researchers who believed that students who are behind in reading actually have a skill deficit—not a developmental lag. (And, as we see in the related article, the skill deficit between average and below-average readers can be largely erased with appropriate early intervention.

References

Click the "References" link above to hide these references.

Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Stuebing, K. K., Shaywitz, B. A., and Fletcher, J. M. (1996). Developmental lag versus deficit models of reading disability: A longitudinal, individual growth curves analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(1), 3-17.

Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first to fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 437-447.

Lyon, G. R., Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Torgesen, J. K., Wood, F. B., Schulte, A., and Olson, R. (2001). Rethinking learning disabilities. In Finn, C. E., Rotherham, A. J., and Hokanson, C. R. (Eds.), Rethinking Special Education For a New Century, Washington, D.C.: Progressive Policy Institute and Thomas B. Fordham Foundation.

Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., Holahan, J. M., Schneider, A. E., Marchione, K. E., Stuebing, K. K., Francis, D. J., Pugh, K. R., and Shaywitz, B. A. (1999). Persistence of dyslexia: The Connecticut longitudinal study at adolescence. Pediatrics, 104(6), 1351-1359.

Reprinted with permission from the Fall 2004 issue of American Educator, the quarterly journal of the American Federation of Teachers.

Reprints

For any reprint requests, please contact the author or publisher listed.
Reading Topics A-Z: About Reading  |  Basics  |  Intervention and Prevention  |  Parent Tips  |  Struggling Readers

Comments

The point not addressed here is: why do consider basic reading 'normal' when entering 1st grade? In England and Russia, it is normal for 4 year olds. In Norway it is normal for children of 7.

My daughter had a reading problem and didn't read well until she was 12. She struggled a lot with spelling but has now completed two degrees, one in dance and one in occupational therapy. She found the reading hard work but not impossible. She has also improved considerably in her spelling. I'm just posting this so that people with older kids don't give up hope. Also our daughter was home schooled (as were all our children).

In response to Laura's G post: Those in Finland are also 7, but what so many don't realise when positing such a late age is what happens BEFORE school entry. In Finland almost all kids at 7 and formal school entry are already literate - and reading, thanks to quality care centres prior to school entry

European countries: 1)culturally VALUE education (in the US, anti-education jokes, cartoons and movies abound); 2)have high-quality, mandatory kindergarten, and; 3)they have high-quality, government-subsidized child care & after-school programs for the entire country - not just for low- or high-income qualifiers. Their children start school on a more equal playing field.

This study is interesting. When I was in first grade and every grade after I had poor reading and math skills and was placed in ld classes for math and reading. After high-school I started reading for fun and my reading level went from very poor to above college level in a years time. I think it also has to do with giving kids reading material they want to read and positive enforcement instead of placing them in ld classes. Kids know they are different from their peers and a lot of times stop trying because they feel so behind.

I agree that poor readers (late bloomers) lag behind. However, I do believe that just as educators must open their minds and keep up with the times, I also believe that educators (and researchers alike) must open their minds to the fact that it isn't just one skill (phonemic awareness) that is lacking. According to Stanislas Dehaine (and others) there are some students for whom phonemic skills are NOT the issue. According to these researchers those students have visual perceptual skills that are in conflict with reading. So we need to acknowledge that subset of students as well.

Another narrow study with another narrow outcome - encouragement, modelling, learning environment, freedom to choose content, freedom to choose context (learning out of school) labelling (negative for non readers) strange priorities and expectations(academics to the exclusion of other skills), funding, time allocation. So many variables that do not fit our one size fits all education (for slaves) model. Would love to see a large scale longitudinal study showing what makes a 'great' reader and - perhaps more importantly - what makes a truly effective human being that can think for themselves and maybe even conduct truly valuable research that lights the way.

The claim suggested in the title (that poor readers will never catch up) just isn't supported by the evidence provided. The fact that a poor reader in the first grade is likely a poor reader three years later in fourth grade does not mean they never catch up or just "wilt." Fourth grade is hardly the end of a child's education, and as the comment from "Layliya" above mentions, some students will increase their reading skills sharply much later on. I teach college English, and yes, even older student are quite capable of improving their reading skills markedly with practice. A big impediment for older students with poor reading skills is that they have been exposed to many teachers who treat them as though they are a lost cause. Actually, recent research has shown the brain remains highly plastic for language into puberty and is still quite plastic after that. Also, someone else mentioned that children in Finland enter school literate at 7 because they learn to read in preschool. That is not at all correct. Reading is not taught in Finnish preschools. That is why academics are so interested in their schooling model--because their children spend so much less time on academics and testing and yet ultimately outscore everyone else.

Add comment

Plain text

  • No HTML tags allowed.
  • Web page addresses and e-mail addresses turn into links automatically.
  • Lines and paragraphs break automatically.
By submitting this form, you accept the Mollom privacy policy.

Our Literacy Blogs

Dr. Joanne Meier
Dr. Joanne Meier
February 14, 2014
Maria Salvadore
Maria Salvadore
September 14, 2017
"You know you’ve read a good book when you turn the last page and feel a little as if you have lost a friend." — Paul Sweeney